Lisa Zunshine was asking the Narrative List about instances of
narratives they find depressing, or reasons why they find them
depressing. I might have suggested Beckett (How It Is comes to
mind) but I named Cormac McCarthy's The
Road and Jonathan Littell's Les
Bienveillantes. Others gave other titles, such as Thomas Hardy's
novels, Ishiguro's Never Let Me Go, Ian McEwan's On Chesil
Beach, Kafka, King Lear, Dostoevsky's
Crime and Punishment... Here
follow two interesting answers, and my second answer at the end.
Marshall Gregory:
Dear Professor Zunshine,
I think the question you posed
below to the Narrative group is quite interesting, and I have been
fascinated by the many responses to it, fascinated in part by how
quickly the responses devolved into "like this" or "don't like this"
evaluations of the sort we generally deplore from our undergraduates,
and how few responses, if any, gave you the kinds of data that would
help you answer your question of whether there is any general principle
of psychological or ethical response to literary representations that
would be predictably depressing, not just ad hoc depressing for this or
that individual.
I also think it interesting that
no one referred to Matthew Arnold's notion in "Preface to Poetry" that
there is indeed a recognizable principle of depression. He is quite
clear about the nature of the principle.
"What then are the situations from
the representations of which, though accurate, no poetical enjoyment
can be derived? They are those in which the suffering finds no vent in
action; in which a continuous state of mental distress is prolonged,
unrelieved by incident, hope, or resistance; in which there is
everything to be endured, nothing to be done. In such situations there
is inevitably something morbid, in the description of them something
monotonous. When they occur in actual life, they are painful, not
tragic; the representation of them in poetry is painful also."
This sounds like a perfect
account, to me, of why so many of your respondents find this literary
representation, or another one, "depressing."
Of course, Arnold goes on to make
a point about this issue that is primarily ethical, not psychological.
The persistent ingestion of literary representations that are
depressing in the sense that he defines, he says, is not good for us as
moral and ethical agents in the world. It undermines our willingness to
believe and act as if the things we believe and the things we do really
matter. This kind of depression, a kind of depression that Arnold
assumes we can "catch" from literature, like catching a cold, cheapens
life and forestalls effective human effort.
I am interested in ethical
criticism myself. I don't know if you have seen my 2009 book from the
U. of Notre Dame P Shaped By Stories: The Ethical Power of
Narratives in the context of which I don't give Arnold much credit for
being a really good ethical critic, but it does seem to me that on this
narrower psychological issue of which stories people find depressing
(and especially on the issue of why,) Arnold is spot on.
Much later in the semester (in my
Literary Criticism class), my students will be reading your "Why Do We
Read Fiction?", so thank you for this fine piece. I'll attach my latest
piece on ethical criticism that appeared last fall in The Journal of
Literary Theory.
H. Porter Abbott:
A few more thoughts, Lisa. One is
that we may be the wrong group to ask. We are as a group inclined and
trained to move smoothly into the analytical mode, which is a way of
displacing depression with 1) something interesting to occupy the mind
(ftnt: a common characteristic of clinical depression is the way it
sucks all the interest out of things) and 2) a sense of power over the
text (re: Allison's comment on powerlessness).
Another is that I don't think you
will get a thematic answer to your question -- witness the variety of
responses so far, sometimes regarding the same text.
And finally, my hunch is that what
counts is the success with which the narrative counterbalances its
depressing content *for a particular reader.* I think something like
this chemistry, as it works out in the mind of any particular reader,
is what releases that reader from the depressing content. This
counterbalancing can be affected by any number of things: management of
the plot, the comparative grace of language, how the material is
focalized, a sense of striking originality, catharsis. Depends on who
you are and how you read/view. It depends even on how much
counterbalancing you need. Some depressive types are half in love with
sinking into a swamp of the irremediably, irredeemably depressing.
Whatever, this gets us away from
personal votes (e.g., "This book really depresses me" -- "What? I found
it wonderful!" etc.). So, in my own case, for one example, feel-good
stories generally depress me. Why? Because they're not convincing. I
don't buy the end of Crime and Punishment. On the other hand, Kafka's
The Trial is a great treat, but because the irremediable/irredeemable
is counterbalanced by a feeling of awe for the book's originality.
Stewart O'Nan's Songs for the Missing takes the worst thing that can
happen to a parent and for many his treatment performs the
counterbalancing magic. Not for me. On the other hand, Laura Lippman's
I'd Know You Anywhere devastates others but succeeds for me. The point
here is not my votes on these texts, but what makes me, and those
unlike me, vote in the different ways we do.
J. A. García Landa:
Then there's the particular context or frame (interactional,
critical) in which the text is being discussed or considered. Looking
at a black canvas with a glimmer of light, some people will turn their
attention to the predominant black, which is justifiable, while others
will take that for granted (both as a matter of fact and as a point
already made) and will inevitably point to the glimmering light as
striking the most significant note in the picture. There's no way the
issue can be completely de-contextualized, least of all in this kind of
context, with theorists lurking behind the screens. There are a number
of frameworks in anthropological criticism to describe what is
"depressing" in general terms—from Northrop Frye's autumn and winter
cycles to contemporary analyses of aesthetics from evolutionary and
sociobiological viewpoints. But then the context varies, and the frame,
and the frame is more important than the picture. If you're looking for
the general anthropological structures, go by all means to Northrop
Frye's demonic imagery, or Jung, or Gilbert Durand's Anthropological Structures of the
Imaginary. But the modulation added by any particular response has to
be studied in context, and it's individual by definition, not even the
reader's but individual in the sense of the interactional event.
____
PS, on Samuel Beckett's pessimism,
No hay comentarios:
Publicar un comentario
Se aceptan opiniones alternativas, e incluso coincidentes: